Provide an in-depth summary of the article.

The APA Style Summary is detailed below.

Provide an in-depth summary of the article.  This must include the who, what, when, where, and how of the article.  The introduction

submitted in PART 3 (above) was the why.  You will detail the method of selection of the subjects of the research, and when and where the research was done.

You must then detail the hypothesis(es) (and there may be more than one), the Research Methods and Research Design used, the results of the research, and the

authors’ conclusions.  Use clear and concise terms, explain jargon, and do not use abbreviations unless they are well-known such as U.S.A.  For all information

included in the introduction and summary, citations must be used including both parenthetical and non-parenthetical citations.  You will only cite the author(s) of

the article you read, and this must match your reference page.  A minimum of two to three full pages is expected.

1 3

Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-1014-y

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

Genetic and environmental influences on conduct and antisocial personality problems in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood

Laura W. Wesseldijk1,2 · Meike Bartels1,2,3 · Jacqueline M. Vink4 · Catharina E. M. van Beijsterveldt1 · Lannie Ligthart1,2 · Dorret I. Boomsma1,2,3 · Christel M. Middeldorp1,3,5

Received: 10 January 2017 / Accepted: 3 June 2017 / Published online: 21 June 2017 © The Author(s) 2017. This article is an open access publication

Based Assessment, were available for 9783 9–10-year-old, 6839 13–18-year-old, and 7909 19–65-year-old twin pairs, respectively; 5114 twins have two or more assessments. At all ages, men scored higher than women. There were no sex differences in the estimates of the genetic and envi- ronmental influences. During childhood, genetic and envi- ronmental factors shared by children in families explained 43 and 44% of the variance of conduct problems, with the remaining variance due to unique environment. During adolescence and adulthood, genetic and unique environ- mental factors equally explained the variation. Longitudi- nal correlations across age varied between 0.20 and 0.38 and were mainly due to stable genetic factors. We conclude that shared environment is mainly of importance during childhood, while genetic factors contribute to variation in conduct and antisocial personality problems at all ages, and also underlie its stability over age.

Keywords Conduct problems · Antisocial personality problems · Longitudinal · Heritability · Shared environment · Twins

Introduction

Conduct problems in children and adolescents and anti- social personality problems in adults involve a variety of repetitive and persistent behaviors that violate the rights of others or societal norms or rules, such as aggression to peo- ple, destruction of property, theft, or violations of rules [1]. Conduct problems during childhood may be the develop- mental precursor for adult antisocial personality problems and are significantly associated with adverse adult out- comes related to health, crime and conviction, and financial and personal functioning [2–6]. Worldwide, childhood, and

Abstract Conduct problems in children and adolescents can predict antisocial personality disorder and related problems, such as crime and conviction. We sought an explanation for such predictions by performing a genetic longitudinal analysis. We estimated the effects of genetic, shared environmental, and unique environmental factors on variation in conduct problems measured at childhood and adolescence and antisocial personality problems measured at adulthood and on the covariation across ages. We also tested whether these estimates differed by sex. Longitudi- nal data were collected in the Netherlands Twin Register over a period of 27 years. Age appropriate and compara- ble measures of conduct and antisocial personality prob- lems, assessed with the Achenbach System of Empirically

This article is part of the focused issue ‘Conduct Disorder and Aggressive Behaviour in Children and Adolescents’.

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00787-017-1014-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

* Laura W. Wesseldijk l.w.wesseldijk@vu.nl

1 Department of Biological Psychology, VU University Amsterdam, Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands

2 EMGO+ Institute for Health and Care Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

3 Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

4 Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

5 Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, GGZ inGeest/VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

1124 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

adolescent conduct problems and adult antisocial personal- ity problems pose a challenge to societies and health care, with prevalence rates ranging between 1 and 4% in the general population [7–9]. It is important to get insight into risk factors for conduct and antisocial personality prob- lems, especially into the factors influencing the stability over age. We, therefore, present a longitudinal twin study (N = 17,513 twin pairs) following twins from age 8 until adulthood (30 years on average).

Twin studies allow the estimation of influences of genetic, shared, and unique environmental factors on indi- vidual differences in behavior and on stability over ages. Most twin studies performed cross-sectional analyses on conduct and antisocial personality problems. Meta-analyses of these studies have estimated the proportion of the varia- tion explained by genetic factors to be between 32 and 60% for children, between 45 and 50% for adolescents, and one meta-analysis provided a heritability estimate of 49% for adults [10–13]. The contribution of the shared environment, also referred to as the ‘common’ familial environment, has been estimated between 10 and 20% in childhood, between 10 and 17% in adolescence, and 14% in adulthood [10–13]. In twin studies, the remaining variation, that is not due to genetics or the shared environment, is attributed to unique environmental influences, which also includes measure- ment error. Thus, all meta-analyses agree upon the impor- tance of genetic factors, and also agree upon a lower esti- mate for the contribution of shared environment.

Longitudinal twin studies on conduct and antisocial per- sonality problems reported stability over ages to be mainly due to genetic factors [14–17], and three studies reported an additional small influence of the shared environment on the stability across ages [15–17]. However, research on antisocial personality problems in later adulthood, i.e., above 24 years, is underrepresented. Furthermore, the sex differences in prevalences for conduct and antisocial per- sonality problems [16, 18] lead to the questions whether the influence of genetic and environmental influences also is different for males and females (i.e., quantitative sex differ- ences) and whether there the same or different genes play a role (i.e., qualitative sex differences). Some twin studies have reported qualitative sex differences for conduct and antisocial problems [16, 19], while others have not [15, 17, 20–23], just as some studies have detected quantitative sex differences [15, 17, 19, 21, 24], and others have not [12, 16, 20, 22, 25]. As the effect of sex might differ across age, we address this question by analyzing large samples of twins ranging from 8 to 65 years.

The aim of the present study was to elucidate the genetic architecture of conduct and antisocial personality problems by analyzing a large longitudinal data set with observations of 9–10-year-old twins (childhood), 13–18-year-old twins (adolescence), and 19–65-year-old twins (adulthood). The

data, collected over a 27 year period in the Netherlands Twin Register, offer the opportunity to examine the fol- lowing questions: (1) what is the heritability of conduct problems in childhood and adolescence and of antisocial personality problems in adulthood? (2) What is the longi- tudinal stability across age and which factors contribute to the stability? (3) Do genetic and environmental factors interact with sex? We analyzed the DSM-oriented conduct problems scales of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and the Youth Self-Report (YSR) and the antisocial person- ality problem scale of the Adult Self-Report (ASR) belong- ing to the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assess- ment (ASEBA) that consist of similar sets of items across ages [26, 27].

Methods

Subjects

The Netherlands Twin Register (NTR) includes a register for young twins, the YNTR [28], and for adult twins, the ANTR [29]. Since 1986, parents can register their young twins shortly after birth with the YNTR and will then receive a survey when their twins are around 1, 2, 3, 5, 7, 10, and 12 years. Between 2005 and 2013, twins themselves were asked to complete a survey when they reached ages 14 and 16 years. At the age of 18 years, twins enroll into the ANTR. The ANTR started in 1991 by recruiting ado- lescent twins and their family members through city coun- cils and subsequently added adults through volunteer regis- tration [29]. The NTR data collection is prospective, with data collected when twins reach a particular age. Therefore, more data are available from twin pairs at younger ages in the YNTR. Due to financial constraints, there was no data collection of the survey for 10 years in 2008 [28]. Until 2008, the survey for 10 years was mailed to both parents of twins around the 10th birthday of the twins. From 2009, the survey was mailed around the 9th birthday of the twins. For the current study, maternal ratings of 9–10-year-old young twins from birth cohorts 1986–2004 were included. Average age was 10 years. The sample included 60 pairs between age 8.7–9 years and 10 pairs who had reached age 12 years when their parents completed the survey, through- out the paper, we refer to this group as 9–10-year-olds. Paternal reports were not included; an earlier study of these twins at age 7 showed that heritability estimates for con- duct problems did not differ between paternal and mater- nal reports [30]. For the 13–18-year-old adolescent twins, self-report data from birth cohorts 1986–1999 were ana- lyzed. Self-report data from 19- to 65-year-old adult twins were collected in the ANTR in 1997, 2000, 2009–2012, or 2013–2014. If adolescent twins had completed multiple

1125Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

surveys, the survey completed by both twins closest to age 16 was selected. For adult twins, a preference was given to the survey that was completed by both twins, closest to age 40. The final sample contained 17,513 twin pairs, including 9783 child twin pairs (9702 complete and 81 incomplete twin pairs on average 10 years), 6839 adolescent twin pairs (5107 complete and 1732 incomplete twin pairs, on aver- age 15.77 years), and 7909 adult twin pairs (4752 complete and 3157 incomplete twin pairs on average 29.39 years). Table 2 presents sample sizes per zygosity-by-sex group. There were 3283 complete and 694 incomplete twin pairs with data in childhood and adolescence. Between adoles- cence and adulthood, the overlap was 1135 complete and 1163 incomplete twin pairs. For 1412 complete and 1253 incomplete twin pairs, the NTR had data in childhood and adulthood. Overall, there were 985 complete and 1913 incomplete twin pairs with data available in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.

Phenotypes

Conduct and antisocial personality problems were meas- ured with the age appropriate versions of the questionnaires belonging to the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA), i.e., the Child Behavior Check- list (CBCL), the Youth Self-Report (YSR), and the Adult Self-Report (ASR). In all three instruments, items are rated on a three-point scale (0–2; not true, somewhat true, very true). The conduct problem scales of the CBCL and YSR are based on a similar set of items that only differ in the phrasing depending on whether the parent is asked to rate his or her child (“Gets in many fights”) or whether the ado- lescent is asked to rate his or her own behavior (“I get in many fights”) (see Supplementary Table 1 for the items included). Items of the ASR used to calculate antisocial personality problems in adults differ between versions over the years of data collection. For this study, we summed the 15 items that were available for all ASR questionnaires obtained in the NTR from 1997 until 2013 (see Supple- mentary Table for the items included). Petersen et al. [31] showed for externalizing problems measured by the CBCL, YSR, and ASR that there is theoretical and empirical sup- port for construct validity invariance and, therefore, for examining these measurements over time.

Statistical analyses

Mean age was calculated in SPSS (version 21). Aver- age symptoms scores and their standard deviations were calculated using OpenMx [34]. The scores for childhood and adolescent conduct and adult antisocial personality problems were highly skewed, as is common for psychi- atric symptoms in population-based samples. Therefore,

to obtain accurate parameter estimations, the scores were divided into three roughly equal sized categories (low, mid- dle, and high scores), and analyzed as categorical data with two thresholds [32]. In a threshold model, where the mean of the distribution is standardized at zero and the standard deviation one, it is assumed that the categorical trait has an underlying continuous distribution of liability [33] and polychoric correlations between twins reflect the correla- tions in liability. Polychoric twin correlations were esti- mated for MZm, DZm, MZf, DZf, and DOS pairs for child- hood, adolescence, and adulthood using structural equation modeling in OpenMx [34]. With the full information maxi- mum likelihood option, all available data were analyzed, including the data from the incomplete twin pairs. We estimated the 95% confidence interval around the correla- tions in OpenMx [35]. Sex differences in the prevalence of conduct and antisocial problems behavior at each age were investigated by testing whether thresholds could be con- strained to be equal across sex. Next, we tested for quan- titative sex effects on twin correlations by constraining the correlations of the male same-sex MZ and DZ twin pairs to be equal to the correlations of the female same-sex MZ and DZ twin pairs, respectively. If these constraints are not allowed, this indicates that the contribution of genetic and environmental influences may differ in males and females. Finally, to investigate whether different genes or different shared environmental factors operate in males and females, we tested whether the correlation for DZ same-sex and DOS twins could be constrained as a function of the DZ same-sex correlations.

Based on the outcomes of these analyses, we proceeded with the longitudinal analyses. First, the correlations between twin1 and twin2 across the three ages (i.e., cross- twin-cross-age correlations) were calculated. Next, in a genetic structural equation model, the observed phenotypic variance in each age group as well as the phenotypic covar- iance across age was partitioned into additive genetic (A), common environmental (C), and non-shared environmen- tal (E) components [36]. MZ twins share (nearly) all their genetic material [37, 38], while DZ twins share, on average, 50% of their segregating genes. Therefore, a higher MZ than DZ twin correlation indicates that genetic factors play a role. When the DZ twin correlations are higher than half of the MZ twin correlation, there is resemblance among twins from the same family that is attributable to common environmental influences shared by children from the same family. Variation that is not due to genes or the common environment shared by twins is attributed to unique envi- ronment. In a similar vein, the genetic, shared environmen- tal, and unique environmental influences to the stability of conduct and antisocial personality problems across the ages were estimated based on the cross-twin-cross-age corre- lations [39]. We derived the estimates for the heritability,

1126 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

the environmental effects, and the correlations between the genetic and environmental factors across the ages from the cross-twin-cross-age correlations after testing for the sig- nificance of A and C by the likelihood-ratio test by com- paring an ACE model to an AE model for children, adoles- cents, and adults. In the likelihood-ratio test, the negative log-likelihood (−2LL) of the more constrained submodel is subtracted from the −2LL of the more general model. The difference between the two models follows a χ2 distribu- tion, where the number of df (degrees of freedom) is equal to the difference in df between the two models. Constraints were retained when they did not significantly deteriorate the fit (p < 0.01 due to multiple testing), so that the most parsimonious model is selected.

Results

Descriptives

Table 1 provides the mean ages, standard deviations, and age ranges of the twins included in the childhood and ado- lescent conduct problems groups and in the adult antiso- cial personality problems group. The untransformed mean symptom scores, standard deviations, and the two thresh- olds for the liability distributions for the three age groups are given for boys and girls separately. As expected, at all ages, males scored higher, which is reflected by signifi- cant differences in the thresholds (p < 0.001, model 2 in Table 3).

Table 1 Mean age, standard deviations (SD), and age range for children, adolescents, and adults

Childhood conduct problems were measured by the CBCL, adolescent conduct problems were measured by the YSR, and adult antisocial per- sonality problems were measured by the ASR. The lower part shows the untransformed mean symptom scores, the standard deviations (SD), and thresholds (Th1 and Th2) based on an underlying normal distribution of liability estimated for the three age groups and separately for males and females

CBCL Child Behavior Checklist, YSR Youth Self-Report, ASR Adult Self-Report

Conduct problems Antisocial personality problems

Children, CBCL Adolescents, YSR Adults, ASR

Mean age (SD) 10.00 (0.44) 15.77 (1.31) 29.39 (11.12)

Min–max 8.71–12.98 13.00–18.00 18.00–64.98

M SD Th1 Th2 M SD Th1 Th2 M SD Th1 Th2

Males 1.57 2.39 −0.10 0.42 3.10 2.58 −0.57 0.38 1.84 2.24 −0.45 0.62 Females 0.85 1.53 0.28 0.82 2.38 2.23 −0.22 0.73 1.51 1.90 −0.33 0.80

Table 2 Sample sizes and the polychoric twin correlations per age-by-zygosity-by-sex group, as well as correlation estimates constrained to be the same across sex (for MZ and DZ twin pairs)

The overlapping sample sizes across age and the cross-twin-cross-age correlations are depicted at the bot- tom of the table, where the MZ correlations are below the diagonal and the DZ correlations above the diagonal

Conduct problems Antisocial personality problems

Children Adolescents Adults

N Twin correlation N Twin correlation N Twin correlation

MZm 1656 0.90 (0.87–0.91) 1012 0.47 (0.40–0.54) 1057 0.44 (0.35–0.51)

DZm 1587 0.68 (0.64–0.72) 928 0.32 (0.22–0.41) 763 0.34 (0.22–0.45)

MZf 1880 0.85 (0.83–0.88) 1429 0.52 (0.45–0.57) 2388 0.41 (0.35–0.46)

DZf 1466 0.66 (0.61–0.71) 1201 0.27 (0.18–0.35) 1481 0.22 (0.14–0.29)

DOS 3194 0.65 (0.61–0.68) 2269 0.21 (0.14–0.27) 2220 0.24 (0.14–0.34)

MZ 0.88 (0.86–0.89) 0.50 (0.45–0.54) 0.42 (0.37–0.46)

DZ 0.66 (0.63–0.69) 0.25 (0.20–0.29) 0.25 (0.19–0.31)

Children 9783 – 0.11 (0.09–0.11) 0.11 (0.09–0.11)

Adolescents 3977 0.18 (0.17–0.19) 6839 – 0.16 (0.13–0.16)

Adult 2665 0.22 (0.21–0.23) 2298 0.30 (0.30–0.31) 7909 –

1127Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

Twin correlations

Polychoric twin correlations and their 95% confidence intervals are shown in Table 2. For all three age groups, MZ correlations were higher than DZ correlations, sug- gesting that additive genetic factors play a role. The DZ correlations were larger than half the MZ correla- tions during childhood, suggesting shared environmen- tal effects. During adolescence and adulthood, the DZ correlations were larger than half of the MZ correla- tions for males, but not for females. However, further testing showed that there were no significant differences (p > 0.01) between the correlations for same-sex male and female twin pairs (i.e., there were no quantitative sex differences), or between the DZ same-sex and DOS twins (i.e., there were no qualitative sex differences) as can be seen from Model 3 and 4 in Table 3. MZ and DZ correla- tions in the most parsimonious model are depicted in the middle of Table 2. The phenotypic correlation between childhood and adolescent conduct problems was 0.20, between adolescent conduct problems and adult antiso- cial personality problems 0.38, and between childhood conduct problems and adult antisocial personality prob- lems 0.22. Table 2 also gives the cross-twin-cross-age correlations for the MZ twins (below the diagonal) and the DZ twins (above the diagonal). These cross-twin- cross-age correlations were higher for MZ twins than for DZ twins, indicating that genetic factors influence the stability of conduct and antisocial personality problems across the ages.

Genetic and environmental influences

An AE model yielded a worse fit than the ACE model in childhood (p < 0.001), while in adolescence and adulthood, the AE model did not lead to a deterioration in fit compared to an ACE model (adolescents: p = 0.55, adults: p = 0.46). The estimates of the proportions of variance explained by genetic and environmental factors, their standard errors, and the genetic and unique environmental correlations across childhood and adolescent conduct problems and adult antisocial personality problems of the final longitudi- nal model are reported in Table 4.

Genetic (43%) and shared environmental (44%) fac- tors were equally important contributors to individual dif- ferences in conduct problems measured in 9–10-year-old twins. During adolescence, the effect of the shared envi- ronment disappeared and genetic influences explained 49% the variance. Roughly similar results were obtained in adulthood, with a heritability estimate of 43%. The effect of the unique environment increased from 13% in child- hood to 51% in adolescence and 57% in adulthood. Genetic and non-shared environmental influences accounted for 91 and 9%, respectively, for the stability between child- hood and adolescent conduct problems. Genetic and non- shared environmental influences accounting for 96 and 4% for the stability between childhood conduct problems and adult antisocial personality problems. Finally, genetic and non-shared environmental influences accounting for 80 and 20% for the stability between adolescent conduct problems and adult antisocial personality problems. These findings

Table 3 Model fitting statistics for the three age groups

For each model, the negative log-likelihood (−2LL) is given, with the number of degrees of freedom (df). The more restrained models are com- pared to models containing a larger number of parameters and tested with a Chi-squared test. The saturated (model 1) includes all correlations, as reported in Table 2. In model 3, the correlations between monozygotic males (MZm) and females (MZf) and the dizygotic males (DZm) and females (DZf) were constrained to be equal (quantitative sex differences). In model 4, the correlations between dizygotic same-sex (DZ) and opposite-sex (DOS) correlations were constrained to be equal (qualitative sex differences)

Estimated parameters −2LL df Compared to X2 p value

Children 1 Saturated 9 34417.66 19,476 – – –

2 Equal thresholds across sex 7 34896.08 19,478 1 478.43 (2) <0.001

3 MZm = Mzf & DZm = DZf 7 34427.56 19,478 1 9.91 (2) 0.01 4 DZ = DOS 6 34429.02 19,479 3 1.46 (1) 0.23

Adolescents 1 Saturated 9 25285.53 11,937 – – –

2 Equal thresholds across sex 7 25541.73 11,939 1 256.21 (2) <0.001

3 MZm = Mzf & DZm = DZf 7 25287.28 11,939 1 1.75 (2) 0.42 4 DZ = DOS 6 25290.91 11,940 3 3.63 (1) 0.06

Adults 1 Saturated 9 23338.24 11,092 – – –

2 Equal thresholds across sex 7 23378.17 11,094 1 39.93 (2) <0.001

3 MZm = Mzf & DZm = DZf 7 23341.73 11,094 1 3.49 (2) 0.18 4 DZ = DOS 6 23341.75 11,095 3 0.02 (1) 0.89

1128 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

correspond with genetic correlations of 0.39 between child- hood and adolescent conduct problems, of 0.67 between adolescent conduct problems and adult antisocial personal- ity problems, and of 0.49 between childhood conduct prob- lems and adult antisocial personality problems (Table 4). The non-shared environmental correlations ranged between 0.03 and 0.14. Thus, there is considerable genetic continu- ity, especially between adolescent conduct problems and adult antisocial personality problems.

Discussion

Our aim was to explore the genetic architecture of conduct and later antisocial personality problems in childhood, ado- lescence problems, and adulthood in a unique longitudinal twin data set, collected over a period of over 27 years. At all ages, we observed the expected sex differences in mean symptom scores, with males scoring higher than females (differences in mean scores equaled about half the stand- ard deviation for children and adolescents and a sixth of the standard deviation for adults). However, no quantitative or qualitative sex differences in genetic architectures were found, i.e., the proportions of variance explained by the genome did not differ between sexes and the same genes seemed to be expressed in males and females. Across ages, we found large differences in the influences of shared and unique environmental factors on variation in conduct and later antisocial personality problems. In 9–10-year-olds, genetic and shared environmental factors were equally important, explaining 43% and 44% of the individual dif- ferences in conduct problems. During adolescence and adulthood, the effect of the shared environment on indi- vidual differences in conduct and antisocial personality problems was non-significant and the genetic and unique environmental effects accounted for 49% and 51% in ado- lescents and 43% and 57% in adults. The phenotypic cor- relations across the ages varied between 0.20 and 0.38, showing childhood and adolescent conduct problems and adult antisocial personality problems are moderately stable. The genetic correlations were substantial across the ages, namely, 0.39 between childhood and adolescent conduct

problems, 0.67 between adolescent conduct problems and adult antisocial personality problems, and 0.49 between childhood conduct problems and adult antisocial personal- ity problems. The unique environmental correlations were far lower, ranging between 0.03 and 0.14.

In line with earlier studies, the heritability of childhood and adolescent conduct problems and adult antisocial per- sonality problems is substantial (between 43% and 49%) [10–13, 40, 41], and genetic factors are the main contributor to covariation across the ages [14–17]. In agreement with an earlier study in 7-year-old Dutch twins, the influence of the shared environment on conduct problems in childhood was large (44%) [30]. Strikingly, the shared environmental effect was non-significant during adolescence and adult- hood, while the sample sizes (6839 adolescent twin pairs and 7909 adult twin pairs, overlap of 3977 twin pairs) were sufficiently large to detect shared environmental influences [42, 43]. This is a different finding than reported by some earlier studies on conduct problems, which found small, at max 23%, but significant shared environmental influences on adolescent conduct problems and adulthood antisocial personality problems [10–17, 40, 44], and on the stability between the ages [15–17]. This difference might be due to differences in the assessment of the phenotype, or may reflect country-shared environment interactions. In Dutch twins, the decrease in the influence of C after childhood has also been reported for anxiety problem [45] and obsessive compulsive problems [46].

How should we interpret the disappearance of the shared environmental influences? It could mean that the shared environment is not of importance anymore after child- hood, for example, because the span of control of the par- ents decreases. We speculate that the shared environmen- tal factors that explain differences in conduct problems during childhood may include factors that have a protec- tive effect that lose their influence during adolescence due to the changed parental role. This speculation is based on the finding that inadequate parental monitoring is a risk factor for the development of child and adolescent con- duct problems [47]. Parental monitoring often decreases from childhood to adolescence as a natural development in the process of raising children [48–52]. A recent study

Table 4 Standardized estimates of additive genetic (A) and common and unique environmental (C and E) influences and their 95% confidence intervals (CI)

Below the diagonal on the right the genetic correlations between the phenotypes assessed in children, ado- lescents and adults are given, and above the diagonal, the unique environmental correlations between the three different ages are presented for the most parsimonious longitudinal model

Model A C E Correlations

Children Adolescents Adults

Children ACE 43% (38–44%) 44% (39–45%) 13% (12–14%) – 0.07 0.03

Adolescents AE 49% (45–51%) – 51% (50–55%) 0.39 – 0.14

Adults AE 43% (39–44%) – 57% (53–61%) 0.49 0.67 –

1129Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

in children-of-twins confirmed that parental knowledge of their children’s whereabouts, activities, and behaviors is a parental influence that diminishes adolescent externalizing behavior after accounting for genetic influences [53]. Con- tinued parental monitoring and parental knowledge may not only be effective during childhood (when the shared environment plays such an important role), but as well dur- ing adolescence.

Future research on environmental factors should con- sider that environmental factors can be correlated with an individual’s genotype, i.e., gene–environment cor- relations. This describes the process, whereby an indi- vidual’s exposure to an environmental factor depends on the individual’s genotype. For example, a preference for peers with externalizing problems can be associated with a genetic predisposition for externalizing problems. For future research on environmental influences on conduct problems, children-of-twins and adoption studies are genetically informative designs that offer possibilities to account for such gene–environment correlations [54, 55].

Another factor, apart from age, that could explain the observed differences in environmental influences throughout development may be the change in rater. Typ- ically, psychopathology in childhood is assessed by par- ents, whereas in adolescence and adulthood, self-ratings are feasible. During childhood, both twins within a twin pair were rated by their mother in our study, whereas in adolescence and adulthood, the twin and co-twin rated themselves; i.e., there were two raters per pair. This change in the number of raters can influence estimates of heritability and the shared environment, as Kan et al. [56] demonstrated. When both twins are rated by the same informant, any rater specific variance is added to the genetic and the shared environmental influences. How- ever, when each twin is rated by a different informant, the rater specific variance is added to the unique environ- mental effect, resulting in a decrease in both the heritabil- ity and the shared environmental estimate. In our current study, we observed only a decrease in the amount of vari- ance explained by shared environment and no decrease in heritability. Thus, our results suggest that the contribution of shared environmental influences may truly decreases with age. A change of rater also raises the question whether the parental and self-reports are measurement invariant, i.e., whether they measure the same underlying trait across age [57]. As shown in Supplementary Table 1, the items included in the CBCL and YSR to assess con- duct problems are highly similar. However, highly similar items do not necessarily imply that the items have identi- cal meaning for mothers and adolescents. To our knowl- edge, no study has addressed construct validity invari- ance for the conduct problem scales of the CBCL and YSR. However, Petersen et al. [31] argued on the basis of

five conditions that there is theoretical and empirical sup- port for construct validity invariance for the externalizing scales of the CBCL and YSR, a scale including all items used in the conduct problem scales (CBCL: 16 out of 33, YSR: 17 out of 30) that were used in the current study [31]. As (1) the measures were derived empirically, (2) showed a similar factor structure across time, (3) showed strong cross-time consistency, (4) strong convergent and discriminant validity over time with respect to internal- izing problems, and (5) the items showed high internal consistency at each age, they concluded that examining the changes in externalizing problems as measured in the CBCL and YSR over time is permitted. Therefore, it seems unlikely that measurement non-invariance between mothers and adolescents fully explains the difference in the estimates for the contribution of C.

Besides the effect of age and the change of rater, the influence of the behavior of one twin on the behavior of the other twin may have become stronger during adolescence. If an increase in problems in one twin causes a decrease in the behavior of the other twin (contrast effect), this can result in an underestimation of the shared environment during adolescence. Twin contrast effects can be detected by analyzing whether there are prevalence differences between the MZ and DZ twins [58, 59]. We did not observe such differences as a function of zygosity: 44.43% of the 9–10-year-old MZ boys scored ‘low’ on conduct problems, compared to 43.64% of the DZ boys (for girls, MZ: 60.6% vs. DZ: 62.17%). For adolescent MZ boys, 29.81% scored ‘low’ vs. 26.76% of the DZ boys (for girls, MZ 44.43% vs. DZ 40.13%) and for adult males, 36.32% scores ‘low’ vs. 31.21% of the DZ males (for females, MZ 37.45% vs. DZ 36.32%). These were the largest differences that were observed and none were significant (p < 0.01 due to mul- tiple testing). For ‘middle’ and ‘high’ scores, the differ- ences in prevalence between MZ and DZ twins were even smaller. Thus, contrast effects between twins also do not appear to have caused an underestimation of the shared environmental influence in any of the age groups.

A limitation of the present study is that the ASEBA questionnaire symptom scores were skewed, as is common for psychiatric symptom scales. We, therefore, analyzed the data with a threshold model, which resulted in more accurate parameter estimates, but in lower statistical power [32]. This was balanced by the large samples, which also provided the opportunity to fully explore sex effects.

In conclusion, this study confirms a substantial genetic influence on conduct and antisocial personality problems across age and an important contribution of the shared environment on childhood conduct problems. There is a moderate stability in conduct problems and antisocial personality problems across the lifespan and genetic fac- tors are the main contributor to this stability over the ages.

1130 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

These findings show the important role of genetic factors across the lifespan and of the shared environment during childhood on conduct problems.

Acknowledgements We thank the members of the twin families reg- istered with the Netherlands Twin Register for their participation.

Compliance with ethical standards

Funding We acknowledge grants from NWO: Twin-family database for behavior genetics and genomics studies (480-04-004); “Spino- zapremie” (NWO/SPI 56-464-14192; “Genetic and Family influences on Adolescent psychopathology and Wellness” (NWO 463-06-001); “A twin-sib study of adolescent wellness” (NWO-VENI 451-04-034); ZonMW “Genetic influences on stability and change in psychopathol- ogy from childhood to young adulthood” (912-10-020), European Research Council (284167), and from the European Union Seventh Framework Program “Aggression in Children: unraveling gene–envi- ronment interplay to inform Treatment and InterventiON strategies” (FP7/2007-2013) under Grant Agreement No. 602768.

Ethical standards The manuscript does not contain clinical studies or patient data.

Conflict of interest On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Crea- tive Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://crea- tivecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statisti- cal manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington

2. Copeland WE, Wolke D, Shanahan L, Costello EJ (2015) Adult functional outcomes of common childhood psychiatric problems: a prospective, longitudinal study. JAMA Psychiatry 72(9):892– 899. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0730

3. Fergusson DM, John Horwood L, Ridder EM (2005) Show me the child at seven: the consequences of conduct problems in childhood for psychosocial functioning in adulthood. J Child Psychol psy- chiatry 46(8):837–849. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00387.x

4. Fergusson DM, Boden JM, Horwood LJ (2010) Classification of behavior disorders in adolescence: scaling methods, predictive validity, and gender differences. J Abnorm Psychol 119(4):699– 712. doi:10.1037/a0018610

5. Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, Angold A (2009) Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66(7):764–772. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.85

6. Copeland WE, Adair CE, Smetanin P, Stiff D, Briante C, Col- man I, Fergusson D, Horwood J, Poulton R, Costello EJ, Angold A (2013) Diagnostic transitions from childhood to adolescence to early adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(7):791–799. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12062

7. Canino G, Polanczyk G, Bauermeister JJ, Rohde LA, Frick PJ (2010) Does the prevalence of CD and ODD vary across cul- tures? Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 45(7):695–704. doi:10.1007/s00127-010-0242-y

8. Bourne R (2010) Antisocial personality disorder: the NICE guideline on treatment, management and prevention. Br J Psy- chiatry 197(4):337. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080440

9. McCollister KE, French MT, Fang H (2010) The cost of crime to society: new crime-specific estimates for policy and pro- gram evaluation. Drug Alcohol Depend 108(1–2):98–109. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.12.002

10. Burt SA (2009) Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: a meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychol Bull 135(4):608–637. doi:10.1037/a0015702

11. Ferguson CJ (2010) Genetic contributions to antisocial per- sonality and behavior: a meta-analytic review from an evo- lutionary perspective. J Soc Psychol 150(2):160–180. doi:10.1080/00224540903366503

12. Rhee SH, Waldman ID (2002) Genetic and environmental influ- ences on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adop- tion studies. Psychol Bull 128(3):490–529

13. Polderman TJ, Benyamin B, de Leeuw CA, Sullivan PF, van Bochoven A, Visscher PM, Posthuma D (2015) Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin stud- ies. Nat Genet. doi:10.1038/ng.3285

14. Hicks BM, Blonigen DM, Kramer MD, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG, Mcgue M (2007) Gender differences and develop- mental change in externalizing disorders from late adolescence to early adulthood: a longitudinal twin study. J Abnorm Psychol 116(3):433–447. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.116.3.433

15. Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (2002) Sex differences in the genetic and environmental influences on the development of antisocial behavior. Dev Psychopathol 14(2):395–416

16. Meier MH, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Martin NG (2011) Sex dif- ferences in the genetic and environmental influences on child- hood conduct disorder and adult antisocial behavior. J Abnorm Psychol 120(2):377–388. doi:10.1037/a0022303

17. Tuvblad C, Eley TC, Lichtenstein P (2005) The development of antisocial behaviour from childhood to adolescence—a longitu- dinal twin study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 14(4):216–225. doi:10.1007/s00787-005-0458-7

18. Moffitt TE (2001) Sex differences in antisocial behaviour : con- duct disorder, delinquency, and violence in the Dunedin longitu- dinal study. Cambridge studies in criminology. Cambridge Uni- versity Press, Cambridge

19. Rose RJ, Dick DM, Viken RJ, Pulkkinen L, Kaprio J (2004) Genetic and environmental effects on conduct disorder and alco- hol dependence symptoms and their covariation at age 14. Alco- hol Clin Exp Res 28(10):1541–1548

20. Slutske WS, Heath AC, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Dunne MP, Statham DJ, Martin NG (1997) Modeling genetic and environmental influences in the etiology of conduct disorder: a study of 2,682 adult twin pairs. J Abnorm Psychol 106(2):266–279. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.106.2.266

21. Saudino KJ, Ronald A, Plomin R (2005) The etiology of behav- ior problems in 7-year-old twins: substantial genetic influence and negligible shared environmental influence for parent ratings and ratings by same and different teachers. J Abnorm Child Psy- chol 33(1):113–130. doi:10.1007/s10802-005-0939-7

22. Eaves LJ, Silberg JL, Meyer JM, Maes HH, Simonoff E, Pick- les A, Rutter M, Neale MC, Reynolds CA, Erikson MT, Heath AC, Loeber R, Truett KR, Hewitt JK (1997) Genetics and developmental psychopathology: 2. The main effects of genes and environment on behavioral problems in the Virginia twin study of adolescent behavioral development. J Child Psychol

1131Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

Psychiatry 38(8):965–980. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997. tb01614.x

23. Vink JM, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt TC, van Dongen J, van Beek JH, Distel MA, de Moor MH, Smit DJ, Minica CC, Lig- thart L, Geels LM, Abdellaoui A, Middeldorp CM, Hottenga JJ, Willemsen G, de Geus EJ, Boomsma DI (2012) Sex differ- ences in genetic architecture of complex phenotypes? PLoS One 7(12):e47371. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047371

24. Zheng Y, Cleveland HH (2015) Differential genetic and envi- ronmental influences on developmental trajectories of antiso- cial behavior from adolescence to young adulthood. J Adolesc 45:204–213. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.10.006

25. Gelhorn HL, Stallings MC, Young SE, Corley RP, Rhee SH, Hewitt JK (2005) Genetic and environmental influ- ences on conduct disorder: symptom, domain and full- scale analyses. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46(6):580–591. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00373.x

26. RescorlaL A (2003) Manual for the ASEBA adult forms and pro- files : for ages 18–59: adult self-report and adult behavior check- list. ASEBA, Burlington

27. Achenbach TM, Rescorla L (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles : an integrated system of multi- informant assessment. ASEBA, Burlington

28. van Beijsterveldt CE, Groen-Blokhuis M, Hottenga JJ, Franic S, Hudziak JJ, Lamb D, Huppertz C, de Zeeuw E, Nivard M, Schutte N, Swagerman S, Glasner T, van Fulpen M, Brouwer C, Stroet T, Nowotny D, Ehli EA, Davies GE, Scheet P, Orle- beke JF, Kan KJ, Smit D, Dolan CV, Middeldorp CM, de Geus EJ, Bartels M, Boomsma DI (2013) The Young Netherlands Twin Register (YNTR): longitudinal twin and family studies in over 70,000 children. Twin Res Hum Genet 16(1):252–267. doi:10.1017/thg.2012.118

29. Willemsen G, Vink JM, Abdellaoui A, den Braber A, van Beek JH, Draisma HH, van Dongen J, van ‘t Ent D, Geels LM, van Lien R, Ligthart L, Kattenberg M, Mbarek H, de Moor MH, Nei- jts M, Pool R, Stroo N, Kluft C, Suchiman HE, Slagboom PE, Geus EJ, Boomsma DI (2013) The Adult Netherlands Twin Reg- ister: twenty-five years of survey and biological data collection. Twin Res Hum Genet 16(1):271–281. doi:10.1017/thg.2012.140

30. Wesseldijk LW, Fedko IO, Bartels M, Nivard MG, van Bei- jsterveldt CE, Boomsma DI, Middeldorp CM (2016) Psy- chopathology in 7-year-old children: differences in maternal and paternal ratings and the genetic epidemiology. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 174(3):251–260. doi:10.1002/ ajmg.b.32500

31. Petersen IT, Bates JE, Dodge KA, Lansford JE, Pettit GS (2015) Describing and predicting developmental profiles of externaliz- ing problems from childhood to adulthood. Dev Psychopathol 27(3):791–818. doi:10.1017/S0954579414000789

32. Derks EM, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI (2004) Effects of censoring on parameter estimates and power in genetic modeling. Twin Res Hum Genet 7(6):659–669. doi:10.1375/1369052042663832

33. Falconer DS, Mackay TFC (1996) Introduction to quantitative genetics. Longmans Green, Harlow, Essex

34. Boker S, Neale M, Maes H, Wilde M, Spiegel M, Brick T, Spies J, Estabrook R, Kenny S, Bates T, Mehta P, Fox J (2011) OpenMx: an open source extended structural equation mod- eling framework. Psychometrika 76(2):306–317. doi:10.1007/ s11336-010-9200-6

35. Neale MC, Miller MB (1997) The use of likelihood-based confi- dence intervals in genetic models. Behav Genet 27(2):113–120. doi:10.1023/A:1025681223921

36. Posthuma D, Beem AL, de Geus EJ, van Baal GC, von Hjelm- borg JB, Iachine I, Boomsma DI (2003) Theory and practice in quantitative genetics. Twin Res Hum Genet 6(5):361–376. doi:10.1375/136905203770326367

37. Zhang R, Thiele H, Bartmann P, Hilger AC, Berg C, Herberg U, Klingmuller D, Nurnberg P, Ludwig M, Reutter H (2016) Whole-exome sequencing in nine monozygotic discordant twins. Twin Res Hum Genet 19(1):60–65. doi:10.1017/thg.2015.93

38. van Dongen J, Slagboom PE, Draisma HHM, Martin NG, Boomsma DI (2012) The continuing value of twin studies in the omics era. Nat Rev Genet 13(9):640–653. doi:10.1038/nrg3243

39. Neale MC, Cardon LR, North Atlantic Treaty Organization. Sci- entific Affairs Division (1992) Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. NATO ASI series Series D, behavioural and social sciences, vol 67. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht

40. Bergen SE, Gardner CO, Kendler KS (2007) Age-related changes in heritability of behavioral phenotypes over adoles- cence and young adulthood: a meta-analysis. Twin Res Hum Genet 10(3):423–433. doi:10.1375/twin.10.3.423

41. Miles DR, Carey G (1997) Genetic and environmental architec- ture of human aggression. J Pers Soc Psychol 72(1):207–217

42. Posthuma D, Boomsma DI (2000) A note on the statistical power in extended twin designs. Behav Genet 30(2):147–158

43. Visscher PM, Gordon S, Neale MC (2008) Power of the classi- cal twin design revisited: II detection of common environmen- tal variance. Twin Res Hum Genet 11(1):48–54. doi:10.1375/ twin.11.1.48

44. Kendler KS, Patrick CJ, Larsson H, Gardner CO, Lichtenstein P (2013) Genetic and environmental risk factors in males for self-report externalizing traits in mid-adolescence and criminal behavior through young adulthood. Psychol Med 43(10):2161– 2168. doi:10.1017/S003329171300007x

45. Lamb DJ, Middeldorp CM, van Beijsterveldt CE, Bartels M, van der Aa N, Polderman TJ, Boomsma DI (2010) Heritabil- ity of anxious-depressive and withdrawn behavior: age-related changes during adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychia- try 49(3):248–255

46. Van Grootheest DS, Bartels M, Van Beijsterveldt CEM, Cath DC, Beekman AT, Hudziak JJ, Boomsma DI (2008) Genetic and environmental contributions to self-report obsessive-compulsive symptoms in Dutch adolescents at ages 12, 14, and 16. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47(10):1182–1188. doi:10.1097/ CHI.0b013e3181825abd

47. Racz SJ, McMahon RJ (2011) The relationship between paren- tal knowledge and monitoring and child and adolescent con- duct problems: a 10-year update. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 14(4):377–398. doi:10.1007/s10567-011-0099-y

48. Laird RD, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA (2003) Parents’ monitoring-relevant knowledge and adolescents’ delin- quent behavior: evidence of correlated developmental changes and reciprocal influences. Child Dev 74(3):752–768. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00566

49. Laird RD, Criss MM, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA (2009) Developmental trajectories and antecedents of dis- tal parental supervision. J Early Adolesc 29(2):258–284. doi:10.1177/0272431608320123

50. Kerr M, Stattin H, Burk WJ (2010) A reinterpretation of parental monitoring in longitudinal perspective. J Res Adolesc 20(1):39– 64. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00623.x

51. Keijsers L, Frijns T, Branje SJT, Meeus W (2009) Developmen- tal links of adolescent disclosure, parental solicitation, and con- trol with delinquency: moderation by parental support. Dev Psy- chol 45(5):1314–1327. doi:10.1037/a0016693

52. Smetana JG (1988) Adolescents and parents concep- tions of parental authority. Child Dev 59(2):321–335. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb01469.x

53. Marceau K, Narusyte J, Lichtenstein P, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Reiss D, Neiderhiser JM (2015) Parental knowledge is an envi- ronmental influence on adolescent externalizing. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 56(2):130–137. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12288

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01614.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01614.x
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0047371
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.10.006
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00373.x
https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2012.118
https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2012.140
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32500
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32500
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000789
https://doi.org/10.1375/1369052042663832
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025681223921
https://doi.org/10.1375/136905203770326367
https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2015.93
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg3243
https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.10.3.423
https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.11.1.48
https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.11.1.48
https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171300007x
https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181825abd
https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181825abd
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0099-y
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00566
https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431608320123
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00623.x
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016693

1132 Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2018) 27:1123–1132

1 3

54. McAdams TA, Neiderhiser JM, Rijsdijk FV, Narusyte J, Lichten- stein P, Eley TC (2014) Accounting for genetic and environmen- tal confounds in associations between parent and child character- istics: a systematic review of children-of-twins studies. Psychol Bull 140(4):1138–1173. doi:10.1037/a0036416

55. O’Connor TG, Deater-Deckard K, Fulker D, Rutter M, Plo- min R (1998) Genotype-environment correlations in late childhood and early adolescence: antisocial behavioral prob- lems and coercive parenting. Dev Psychol 34(5):970–981. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.970

56. Kan KJ, van Beijsterveldt CE, Bartels M, Boomsma DI (2014) Assessing genetic influences on behavior: informant and context

dependency as illustrated by the analysis of attention problems. Behav Genet 44(4):326–336. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9657-7

57. Wicherts JM (2016) The importance of measurement invariance in neurocognitive ability testing. Clin Neuropsychol 30(7):1006– 1016. doi:10.1080/13854046.2016.1205136

58. Carey G (1992) Twin imitation for antisocial behavior: impli- cations for genetic and family environment research. J Abnorm Psychol 101(1):18–25

59. Carey G (1986) Sibling imitation and contrast effects. Behav Genet 16(3):319–341. doi:10.1007/Bf01071314

European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry is a copyright of Springer, 2018. All Rights Reserved.

  • Genetic and environmental influences on conduct and antisocial personality problems in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Methods
      • Subjects
      • Phenotypes
      • Statistical analyses
    • Results
      • Descriptives
      • Twin correlations
      • Genetic and environmental influences
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgements
    • References

"Get 15% discount on your first 3 orders with us"
Use the following coupon
FIRST15

Order Now